INVESTIGATING CLOCK READING SKILLS OF THIRD GRADERS WITH AND WITHOUT DYSCALCULIA RISK
Abstract
This paper aims to investigate the clock reading skills of third graders with and without dyscalculia risk. Data were collected from 290 (136 girls, 154 boys) third graders from three different primary schools located in the city center of Mus. Of the participants, 29 were at risk of dyscalculia while 261 were normally achieving. The study employed the triangulation method, one of the mixed-method research design patterns. Data collection tools include a mathematics achievement test, a clock-reading test developed by the researchers, and an interview form designed for children who are at risk of dyscalculia. Analysis of data from all participants revealed that there is no significant difference between boys’ and girls’ mathematics achievements. There is a significant difference in terms of clock reading skills in favor of boys. There is a moderate (.56) relationship between mathematics achievement and clock reading skills, and ability to read clocks explains 31% of the total variance in mathematics achievement. The means of the scores of children with and without dyscalculia risk from the 28-item clock-reading test were 5.45 and 11, respectively. During the interviews with 20 children at risk of dyscalculia, the majority of children (13) were able to draw an analog clock but only 50% of the children were able to draw the given time correctly on the clock. It was also found that most of the children (12) confused hour and minute hands and could not tell why there are 12 numbers on an analog clock while there are 24 hours in a day (average number of correct answers 8). Besides, only four students gave correct answers to the question regarding five-minute intervals. The results indicate that children with and without dyscalculia risk have difficulty reading clocks. Furthermore, our results show that, considering the predictivity of clock reading skills for mathematics achievement, difficulties in reading clocks at an early age can be considered as an early indicator of dyscalculia in children.
Keywords: Dyscalculia, mathematics-learning difficulty, reading clock, analog clock
REFERENCES
Andersson, U. (2008). Mathematical competencies in children with different types of learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 48-66.
Andersson, U., & Östergren, R. (2012). Number magnitude processing and basic cognitive functions in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(6), 701-714.
Ansari, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Atypical trajectories of number development: A neuroconstructivist perspective. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(12), 511-516.
Baki, A., & Gökçek, T. (2012). Karma yöntem araştırmalarına genel bir bakış [A general overview of mixed method researches]. Electronic Journal of Social Sciences, 11(42), 1-21.
Bender, A. & Beller, S. (2012). Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool. Cognition, 124(2), 156- 182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.005
Boulton-Lewis, G., Wilss, L., & Mutch, S. (1997). Analysis of primary school children’s abilities and strategies for reading and recording time from analogue and digital clocks. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 9(2), 136-151.
Burton, G., & Edge, D. (1985). Helping children develop a concept of time. School Science and Mathematics, 85(2), 109‑120.
Burny, E., Valcke, M., & Desoete, A. (2009). Towards an agenda for studying learning and instruction focusing on time‐related competences in children. Educational Studies, 35(5), 481-492.
Burny, E., Valcke, M., & Desoete, A. (2012). Clock reading: An underestimated topic in children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(4), 351-360.
Butterworth, B. (2005). Developmental dyscalculia. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. (pp.455-468). New York: Psychology Press.
Butterworth, B., & Laurillard, D. (2010). Low numeracy and dyscalculia: Identification and intervention. ZDM Mathematics Education, 42, 527–539.
Case, R., Sandieson, K., & Dennis, S. (1986). Two cognitive-developmental approaches to the design of remedial instruction. Cognitive Development, 1, 293-333.
Clarke, D. (1998). Children’s understanding of the clock in the digital age. Primary Educator, 4(3), 9–12.
Dowker, A. (2009). What works for children with mathematical difficulties? The effectiveness of intervention schemes. Research Report RR554. DfES Publications.
Desoete, A., & Grégoire, J. (2006). Numerical competence in young children and in children with mathematics learning disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences, 16(4), 351-367.
Foreman, N., Boyd-Davis, S., Moar, M., Korallo, L., & Chappell, E. (2008). Can virtual environments enhance the learning of historical chronology?. Instructional Science, 36(2), 155-173.
Friedman, W., & Laycock, F. (1989). Children’s analog and digital clock knowledge. Child Development, 60, 357-371.
Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Morris, R. D., & Lyon, G. R. (2005). Evidence-based assessment of learning disabilities in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34(3), 506-522.
Friedman, W. J., & Laycock, F. (1989). Children's analog and digital clock knowledge. Child Development, 60, 357-371.
Geary, D. C. (1990). A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. Journal of experimental child psychology, 49(3), 363-383.
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 345.
Geary, D.C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37(1), 4-15.
Geary, D. C., & Hoard, M. K. (2005). Learning disabilities in arithmetic and mathematics: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 253-267). New York: Psychology Press
Geary, D. C. (2006). Dyscalculia at an early age: Characteristics and potential influence on socio-emotional development. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development, 15, 1-4.
Geary, D.C. (2010). Mathematical disabilities: Reflections on cognitive, neuropsychological and genetic components. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 130–133.
Greene, J. C., Caracelli, V. J., & Graham, W. F. (1989). Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluation designs. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 11(3), 255-274.
Harris, S. (2008). It's about time: Difficulties in developing time concepts. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 13(1), 28-31.
Hurrell, D. (2017). Is it time to start reconsidering the teaching of time? Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 22(3), 33.
McGuire, L. (2007). Time after time: What is so tricky about time? Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 12(2), 30-32.
McInerney, D. M. & McInerney, V. (2002). Educational psychology: Constructing learning. Sydney: Pearson Education.
Morse, J. M. (1991). Strategies for sampling. Qualitative nursing research: A contemporary dialogue, 127-145.
Piazza, M., Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Berteletti, I., Conte, S., Lucangeli, D., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia. Cognition, 116(1), 33-41.
Shalev, R. S., Manor, O., Kerem, B., Ayali, M., Badichi, N., Friedlander, Y., & Gross-Tsur, V. (2001). Developmental dyscalculia is a familial learning disability. Journal of learning disabilities, 34(1), 59-65.
Sharma, M. C. (2015). A window into dyscalculia and other mathematics difficulties. The Routledge international handbook of dyscalculia and mathematical learning difficulties, 277.
Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., & Ullman, J. B. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (Vol. 5). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Thompson, J.; Wood, C. L.; Test, D. W.; & Cease-Cook, J. (2012). Effects of Direct Instruction on telling time by students with autism. Journal of Direct Instruction, 12, 1-12.
Van de Walle, J. A. (2001). Elementary and middle school mathematics: Teaching developmentally. New York: Longman.
Vakali, M. (1991). Clock time in seven to ten-year-old children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 6(3), 325.
Van Steenbrugge, H., Valcke, M., & Desoete, A. (2010). Mathematics learning difficulties in primary education: Teachers’ professional knowledge and the use of commercially available learning packages. Educational Studies, 36, CP3-71. doi:10.1080/ 03055690903148639.
von Aster, M. G., & Shalev, R. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49(11), 868-873.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2011). WHO classifications F81.2 Specific disorder of Arithmetic skills. Retrieved June 9, 2011, from http://who.int/classifications/icd/en/ bluebook.pdf
Williams, R. F. (2004). Making meaning from a clock. Ph. D Dissertation, University of California.
Williams, A. (2013). A teacher's perspective of dyscalculia: Who counts? An interdisciplinary overview. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 18(1), 1-16.
Wright, S. (2007). Graphic-narrative play: Young children’s authoring through drawing and telling. International Journal of Education and the Arts, 8(8), 1–28.
Downloads
Published
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2021 International Online Journal of Primary Education (IOJPE) ISSN: 1300-915X

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
Copyright and permissions
The manuscripts submitted to International Online Journal of Primary Education (IOJPE) for publication should be original studies that were not published before or not submitted to anywhere else for publication.
Authors who submit their manuscript to International Online Journal of Primary Education (IOJPE) should acknowledge that they agree to transfer the copyright of their studies to IOJPE. All Open Access articles are distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that the original work is properly cited.
All articles published in International Online Journal of Primary Education (IOJPE) are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (CC BY 4.0).
Any further distribution or use of content published under CC BY 4.0 must contain the author(s) and the published article’s title, and journal citation. All articles published in IOJPE under a CC BY License may be used for Text and Data Mining purposes, subject to the conditions of the CC BY License terms. The license allows for commercial use. IOJPE allows reusers to distribute, remix, adapt, and build upon the material in any medium or format, so long as attribution is given to the creator.
The journal’s objective is to disseminate articles published are free. Under the Creative Commons license (CC BY 4.00), the journal allows the user to permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, and even use the publication for commercial activities, provided that the original work is properly cited.
Open access is an approach that eases the interdisciplinary communication and encourages cooperation among different disciplines. IOJPE, therefore, contributes to its own field by providing more access to its articles and a more transparent review process.